The
information is exactly the same, so why would students opt for the pricier and
more cumbersome version? Is the library missing something important about the
nature of printed versus electronic books?
Some
studies do show that information becomes more securely fixed in people’s minds
when they read it from paper than when they read it from the screen (as
summarized in this recent blog post).
Findings like these may resonate with our subjective experience of reading, and
yet still seem puzzling at an intellectual level. This is because we’re used to
thinking about reading—or information processing more generally—as the
metaphorical equivalent of consuming food. We talk about devouring
novels, digesting a report, and absorbing information. If we’re
ingesting the same material, whether it’s presented in print or electronically,
how can the results be so different?
Chew Your Food
Within
the prevailing food metaphor, the only sensible way to think about these
different outcomes is that reading from paper leads to more efficient or
complete digestion. An intuitive explanation may be that visual fatigue or the
effort of navigating text onscreen interferes with the processing of
information. Or, a popular subject of modern hand-wringing, perhaps we’ve
picked up shallow mental habits while onscreen that prevent us from taking the
time to properly chew on the information as we take it in. In both
cases, the implication is that valuable informational nutrients that are
“there” in the text end up being mentally excreted rather then absorbed.
But
in reality, the whole reading-as-digestion metaphor is deeply flawed. Cognitive
research shows that the way we read varies widely in different settings, with
text acting as a prompt for very different kinds of mental pursuits. While
reading, it’s possible, among other things, to generate strong visual images
based on the text, to marshal arguments against the author’s main point, to
speculate about the motivations of characters, to connect the text to personal
experiences, to form an opinion, or to notice the sensory and aesthetic
qualities of the text, to name just a few. Not all of these take place every
time you read, so there is not just one activity called “reading,” done either
poorly or well.
A
growing body of research shows that the same information can trigger very
different thoughts depending on the cognitive goals that people have in mind.
Readers can be instructed to create vivid imagery or
to learn over time to make deeper inferences,
both of which lead to better retention of the material they’ve read. And when
readers are told to form an impression of people they’re reading about rather
than to read for the purpose of memorizing the text, they organize the information from the text less
haphazardly and are able to recall more of it.
Cognitive
goals can also be unintentionally triggered by cues that never even
enter a reader’s awareness. So, just as people can be told to form an
impression of a character they read about, they can also be prompted to
unconsciously pursue the same goal. In one study,
researchers asked people to unscramble sentences that contained words like evaluate,
judgment, and personality before reading excerpts about a
character. In another,
these words were subliminally flashed at subjects before they took part in the
reading task. In both of these studies, simply seeing words related to the goal
of character assessment affected readers in much the same way as asking them
explicitly to judge character.
Information Influx
In
fact there are probably all sorts of subtle cues around us, influencing our
cognitive goals moment by moment. In one experiment,
subjects who subliminally saw the Apple logo performed better on a test of
creativity than those who were exposed to IBM’s logo, possibly because Apple
has been so successful at entwining its brand with the notion of creativity. Another study
showed that when people read a product review in a hard-to-read font, they more
carefully evaluated the merits of the arguments than when the same information
was presented in easy-to-read font—suggesting that when information merely feels
hard to process, we automatically bring out the heavy cognitive machinery.
The
emerging research on cognitive goals and their triggers offers an intriguing
way to think about why reading the same text in different formats or even
styles of presentation might engage the mind in such different ways. A
hard-copy textbook—including its four-pound heft—may serve as a powerful cue
that sets off cognitive activities that are very distinct from those that are
involved in reading your Twitter feed or thumbing through a paperback romance
novel. Through its lifelong associations with classrooms and the intellectual
calisthenics that take place there, a physical tome may spark a self-analytical
frame of mind, prompting you to take stock of your understanding, re-reading
passages to fill in gaps, and constantly “testing” yourself on your mastery of
the material.
The
research should also motivate publishers—especially of online text—to think
deeply about how elements of presentation and design can serve as signals to
nudge the reader into the mental activities that do justice to the text. For
example, an online literary mag that looks like a page from Buzzfeed may leave
readers with limp, unsatisfying experiences simply because it’s too hard to
arouse the contemplative and sensory goals that lead to properly savoring its
content. The magazine needs to signal that a different kind of reading is
called for, perhaps by borrowing some of the elements that poets have long used
to cue readers to pay close attention to the language of a poem:
stripping away graphic distractions, formatting text sparsely and
unconventionally, and surrounding it with generous swaths of empty space.
Understanding how
reading works means abandoning the idea that the presentation of a text is as
inconsequential as whether a plate of food is served with a sprig of decorative
parsley. In fact, the packaging of text likely contains rich implicit
instructions for what we do with it.
No comments:
Post a Comment