It’s Not You It’s Me: If a Dog Won’t Play With You, It
Could Be Your Fault
The views expressed are those of the
author and are not necessarily those of
Scientific American.
You’ve probably heard the
expression, Life Is Short: Play With A Dog. “Okay!” you think. “I’ll do it!”
After all, dogs play together until they are gray in the face. Dogs also play
with people, although that’s not always a given. Have you ever tried to play
with a dog and it just doesn’t work? “The dog’s not playing right,” you may
think. “This stinks.”
Don’t be so quick to blame the dog.
Recent research suggests that it might be you who is not “playing
right.” I initially found the implications of this research rather benign,
extending no further than a particular dog-human duo. But a study published in
2014 found that whether or not a dog plays with a person can be life-changing.
I’m getting ahead of myself. Let’s
start at the beginning.
In 2001, animal welfare and behavior
researcher Nicola Rooney
— now at the University of Bristol — had a simple question about dog-human
play. She and her colleagues wanted to know whether dogs respond to peoples’
play signals. In the study, volunteers played with their dogs for 5 minutes in
the comfort of their homes, and the play sessions were videotaped. Owners were
asked to play with their dog “as they usually did,” but here’s the key: they
were not allowed to use objects or toys.
After the play sessions, the
researchers watched the videos and noted which behaviors owners used to
initiate or maintain play. They identified 35 common play signals, including
patting the floor, clapping, shoving, hitting or tapping the dog, and of
course, play bowing. People also blew at dogs, barked at them, and grabbed
their paws. And who can forget my favorite behavior, ‘hand spider,’ where the
“person moved their hand or fingers simulating movement of an insect or other
creature.”
Identifying peoples’ play behaviors
was just the first step. The real goal was to understand the effect of
these signals: did peoples’ play signals lead dogs to play with their owners?
And more specifically, did the commonly used signals elicit play more often
than the rarely used signals?
Before spilling the beans, I want
you to think for a moment how it feels when a dog plays with you. AWESOME is
the obvious and only answer. It feels AWESOME when you are on the same page
with a dog and engaging in mutual, reciprocal play. I remember a dog who was a
bit wary of me when we first met. I’d walk into a room where she was sitting,
and I’d receive a sideways glance as she slinked out. Ouch. Over time, ignoring
her, letting her sniff me, and dropping treats on the floor paid off. The
slinking and leaving decreased, and one day, we were sitting on the floor, and
I got a play bow! A full-on play bow! IT WAS AWESOME! That is, until I felt a
huge thunk of responsibility. What should I do next? What would be the best
type of playful olive branch to extend to her? Many play behaviors involve
touching, but surely we weren’t at the playful touching stage yet. Clearly I
was overthinking this.
I think I responded with a play bow,
defined as a “posture similar to a canine play bow, with palms and knees in
contact with the ground, forequarters lowered and arms outstretched.” Or maybe
an “abbreviated” play bow, with my hands on my thighs. I didn’t want to be too
loud or too sudden. Knowing me, I definitely had on a play face. Much later,
when we were in the ‘touching is cool’ phase, I remember responding to a play
bow by head-butting her side. I remember thinking, “I don’t know if this will
work, but it’s worth a shot.”
Now that she and I are well into a
stage of playing together, I have a better perspective on what our no-play days
were like. Those days are marked by a haze of trepidation, the realization that
we were figuring out what play would look like for us, and the awareness that
what I threw out there would not necessarily be well-received. I wanted things
to move forward, but even more importantly, I didn’t want things to move back.
I was right to be concerned. Of the
35 most common play signals, Rooney and colleagues found that a signal’s
popularity “was not related to its success at initiating or sustaining play.”
For example, patting the floor was used the most often, but play followed only
38% of the time. It appears patting the floor was not very successful at
initiating or increasing play with a dog. Sad face. Other not-so-successful but
commonly used play behaviors included scruffing the dog and clapping. Some
things people did even elicited play 0% of the time! These included picking up
and kissing the dog as well as stamping one’s feet. These “play” behaviors
produced a net gain of zero.
All is not lost! A few behaviors
were incredibly successful at eliciting play with a dog. Rooney and colleagues
found that chase-and-running-away as well as lunging forward were associated
with play 100% of the time! “Signaling up” (tapping one’s chest to entice the
dog to jump up), grabbing or holding a dog’s paws, and of course, play bowing
were also successful at eliciting play.
The researchers’ conclusion is
somewhat somber: “We suggest that humans often use ineffective [play] signals.”
Instead of blaming dogs for “not playing right,” people could look at their own
behavior and evaluate its effect, acknowledging that certain play signals are
more effective at eliciting play than others.
Bring me a playful dog!
Sometimes we hold dogs responsible when we don’t get the type of play we want, even subconsciously. One way to explore this phenomenon is to look at whether a shelter dog’s play behavior affects whether it will be adopted. Alexandra Protopopova and colleagues at the University of Florida Canine Cognition and Behavior Lab and Arizona State University Canine Science Collaboratory found that when a potential adopter takes out a shelter dog for a one-on-one interaction, only two behavioral variables predicted whether that dog was leaving the shelter: 1) lying in close proximity to the person, and 2) responding to the person’s play solicitation.
Sometimes we hold dogs responsible when we don’t get the type of play we want, even subconsciously. One way to explore this phenomenon is to look at whether a shelter dog’s play behavior affects whether it will be adopted. Alexandra Protopopova and colleagues at the University of Florida Canine Cognition and Behavior Lab and Arizona State University Canine Science Collaboratory found that when a potential adopter takes out a shelter dog for a one-on-one interaction, only two behavioral variables predicted whether that dog was leaving the shelter: 1) lying in close proximity to the person, and 2) responding to the person’s play solicitation.
You heard me right: of all the
things a dog might do when first meeting someone, only two things were
associated with whether a dog was adopted: dogs laying close to the person were
about 14 times more likely to be adopted, and a dog who ignored a person’s play
initiation was unlikely to be adopted.
Taken together, these two studies
paint a potentially scary picture for shelter dogs: people do not always use
play signals that result in play, but people are unlikely to adopt a dog who
does not respond to their play signals. Nobody wins.
When I think about dogs in the
shelter going up for their one-on-one interviews, I hope potential adopters cut
them some slack. The list of factors that could contribute to whether or not a
dog will play with a new, strange human they just met are endless. On top of
that, the shelter environment is often a weird, chaotic land (a complex topic
for a future post). Go slowly. Keep your expectations in check. For shelter
dogs, like speed dating, a lot is riding on the first encounter. Reflect on
your play behaviors just as much as you think about theirs.
—–
Keep up with the researchers whose
work is mentioned above: Alexandra (Sasha) Protopopova (Twitter), Clive Wynne (Twitter)
and John Bradshaw (Twitter).
References and Recommended Reading
Protopopova A. & Wynne C.D.L. (2014). Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption, Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 157 109-116. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.04.007
Protopopova A. & Wynne C.D.L. (2014). Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption, Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 157 109-116. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2014.04.007
Rooney N.J., Bradshaw, J.W.S. &
Robinson, I.H. (2001). Do dogs respond to play signals given by humans?, Animal
Behaviour, 61 (4) 715-722. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1661
Todd, Z. (2014). Adopting Shelter Dogs:
Should Fido Lie Down or Play?
Companion Animal Psychology Blog.
About
the Author: Julie Hecht is a canine behavioral researcher and science
writer in New York City. She would really like to meet your dog.
No comments:
Post a Comment