The Source of Bad
Writing
The 'curse of knowledge' leads writers to
assume their readers know everything they know
By Steven Pinker in
the Wall Street Journal
Why is so much writing
so bad? Why is it so hard to understand a government form, or an academic
article or the instructions for setting up a wireless home network?
The most popular
explanation is that opaque prose is a deliberate choice. Bureaucrats insist on
gibberish to cover their anatomy. Plaid-clad tech writers get their revenge on
the jocks who kicked sand in their faces and the girls who turned them down for
dates. Pseudo-intellectuals spout obscure verbiage to hide the fact that they
have nothing to say, hoping to bamboozle their audiences with highfalutin
gobbledygook.
But the bamboozlement
theory makes it too easy to demonize other people while letting ourselves off
the hook. In explaining any human shortcoming, the first tool I reach for is
Hanlon's Razor: Never attribute to malice that which is adequately explained by
stupidity. The kind of stupidity I have in mind has nothing to do with
ignorance or low IQ; in fact, it's often the brightest and best informed who
suffer the most from it.
I once attended a
lecture on biology addressed to a large general audience at a conference on technology,
entertainment and design. The lecture was also being filmed for distribution
over the Internet to millions of other laypeople. The speaker was an eminent
biologist who had been invited to explain his recent breakthrough in the
structure of DNA. He launched into a jargon-packed technical presentation that
was geared to his fellow molecular biologists, and it was immediately apparent
to everyone in the room that none of them understood a word and he was wasting
their time. Apparent to everyone, that is, except the eminent biologist. When
the host interrupted and asked him to explain the work more clearly, he seemed
genuinely surprised and not a little annoyed. This is the kind of stupidity I
am talking about.
Call it the Curse of
Knowledge: a difficulty in imagining what it is like for someone else not to
know something that you know. The term was invented by economists to help
explain why people are not as shrewd in bargaining as they could be when they
possess information that their opposite number does not. Psychologists
sometimes call it mindblindness. In the textbook experiment, a child comes into
the lab, opens an M&M box and is surprised to find pencils in it. Not only
does the child think that another child entering the lab will somehow know it
contains pencils, but the child will say that he himself knew it contained
pencils all along!
The curse of knowledge
is the single best explanation of why good people write bad prose. It simply
doesn't occur to the writer that her readers don't know what she knows—that
they haven't mastered the argot of her guild, can't divine the missing steps
that seem too obvious to mention, have no way to visualize a scene that to her
is as clear as day. And so the writer doesn't bother to explain the jargon, or
spell out the logic, or supply the necessary detail.
Anyone who wants to
lift the curse of knowledge must first appreciate what a devilish curse it is.
Like a drunk who is too impaired to realize that he is too impaired to drive,
we do not notice the curse because the curse prevents us from noticing it.
Thirty students send me attachments named "psych assignment.doc." I
go to a website for a trusted-traveler program and have to decide whether to
click on GOES, Nexus, GlobalEntry, Sentri, Flux or FAST—bureaucratic terms that
mean nothing to me. My apartment is cluttered with gadgets that I can never
remember how to use because of inscrutable buttons which may have to be held
down for one, two or four seconds, sometimes two at a time, and which often do
different things depending on invisible "modes" toggled by still
other buttons. I'm sure it was perfectly clear to the engineers who designed
it.
Multiply these daily
frustrations by a few billion, and you begin to see that the curse of knowledge
is a pervasive drag on the strivings of humanity, on par with corruption,
disease and entropy. Cadres of expensive professionals—lawyers, accountants,
computer gurus, help-line responders—drain vast sums of money from the economy
to clarify poorly drafted text.
There's an old saying
that for the want of a nail the battle was lost, and the same is true for the
want of an adjective: the Charge of the Light Brigade during the Crimean War is
only the most famous example of a military disaster caused by vague orders. The
nuclear accident at Three Mile Island in 1979 has been attributed to poor
wording (operators misinterpreted the label on a warning light), as have many
deadly plane crashes. The visually confusing "butterfly ballot" given
to Palm Beach voters in the 2000 presidential election led many supporters of
Al Gore to vote for the wrong candidate, which may have swung the election to George
W. Bush, changing the course
of history.
How can we lift the
curse of knowledge? The traditional advice—always remember the reader over your
shoulder—is not as effective as you might think. None of us has the power to
see everyone else's private thoughts, so just trying harder to put yourself in
someone else's shoes doesn't make you much more accurate in figuring out what
that person knows. But it's a start. So for what it's worth: Hey, I'm talking
to you. Your readers know a lot less about your subject than you think,
and unless you keep track of what you know that they don't, you are guaranteed
to confuse them.
A better way to
exorcise the curse of knowledge is to close the loop, as the engineers say, and
get a feedback signal from the world of readers—that is, show a draft to some
people who are similar to your intended audience and find out whether they can
follow it. Social psychologists have found that we are overconfident, sometimes
to the point of delusion, about our ability to infer what other people think,
even the people who are closest to us. Only when we ask those people do we
discover that what's obvious to us isn't obvious to them.
The other way to
escape the curse of knowledge is to show a draft to yourself, ideally
after enough time has passed that the text is no longer familiar. If you are
like me you will find yourself thinking, "What did I mean by that?"
or "How does this follow?" or, all too often, "Who wrote this
crap?" The form in which thoughts occur to a writer is rarely the same as
the form in which they can be absorbed by a reader. Advice on writing is not so
much advice on how to write as on how to revise.
Much advice on writing
has the tone of moral counsel, as if being a good writer will make you a better
person. Unfortunately for cosmic justice, many gifted writers are scoundrels,
and many inept ones are the salt of the earth. But the imperative to overcome
the curse of knowledge may be the bit of writerly advice that comes closest to
being sound moral advice: Always try to lift yourself out of your parochial
mind-set and find out how other people think and feel. It may not make you a
better person in all spheres of life, but it will be a source of continuing
kindness to your readers.
— Mr. Pinker is
Johnstone Professor of Psychology at Harvard University and the chairman of the
Usage Panel of the American Heritage Dictionary. This article is adapted from
his book "The Sense of Style: The Thinking Person's Guide to Writing in
the 21st Century."
No comments:
Post a Comment